Australasian Science: Australia's authority on science since 1938

Getting Stoned May Not Be Making Teens Stupid

A previously reported causal link between teenage marijuana use and a reduction in IQ may be erroneous. A Norwegian researcher re-examined data from the Dunedin longitudinal study of cannabis users in New Zealand and found that the decrease in IQ observed may instead be linked to lower socioeconomic status.

“The approaches differ between these two papers, reflecting the authors’ backgrounds.

“The original paper is led by authors specialising in psychology and psychiatry. They do not consider variables such as socio-economic status (SES), and their statistical analyses rely heavily on strong and untested assumptions. For example, they assume a simple linear relationship between the duration of marijuana smoking and the change in IQ, and their results do not show clearly how confident we can be in the magnitude of the results.

“In contrast, Rogeberg, an economist who authored the more recent study*, takes SES into account, and has a more thorough approach to the statistical methods he applies. He is also clearer about the uncertainty of his results, using simulations to explore the potential effects of different assumptions.

“My perspective as a biostatistician accustomed to working in epidemiological settings is different again.

“Firstly, there is the issue of statistical power. The Dunedin study is not large, with only 874 participants in the original analysis by Meier et al., of whom just 153 ever met the criteria for cannabis dependence and 124 used cannabis regularly at one or more of the five surveys conducted. I suspect that the small sample size limited the extent to which the authors were able to take possible confounding factors, such as SES, into account, and led them to make assumptions such as the linear relationship mentioned above, and assumed parallel effects for different SES groups, genders, etc.

“Secondly, as Rogeberg points out, the data do not support the assumption that a response pattern across different cannabis exposure categories is the same across educational groups. Similarly there are no data shown to support the assumption that the pattern is the same for men and women.

“So are the original results by Meier et al. correct? I don’t believe we can say whether they are or not. In my opinion, their methods of analysis are inadequate as Rogeberg argues. Even if confounding by SES is not substantial, what about gender? Meier et al. show that proportionately more males than females are cannabis users, especially heavy users.

“Even if a more careful, critical analysis were conducted comparing patterns across all the potentially confounding variables (such as SES and gender), it is also important to check the robustness of any assumptions made about the relationship between levels of smoking and effects. In the end it is possible that there would not be sufficient statistical power to detect clinically meaningful differences among groups, even if they exist. Only larger longitudinal studies can answer the question.”

Professor Annette Dobson is Director of the Centre for Longitudinal Lifecourse Research, School of Population Health, University of Queensland and Professor of Biostatistics.

******

“Rogeberg’s challenge is based on simulations, but we used actual data on 1000 people to carry out the analyses he suggested. Rogeberg’s claim is that low SES children’s IQs are temporarily boosted by schooling, but that when they leave school and become adults their IQs rebound down to their former low baseline. If many cannabis users were former low-SES children, Rogeberg says this coincidence could create the false impression that cannabis was responsible for their adult IQ drop.

“Our data show that:

1. Looking at only children from middle-class homes, our findings of IQ decline in adolescent-onset cannabis users remain unaltered. To repeat the research within the middle-class group, we restricted our analysis to individuals in middle-class families (whose primary breadwinner had occupations such as building inspector, plumber or aircraft mechanic). We excluded low-SES families (whose breadwinners had low-skills occupations such as food packer), and we also excluded high-SES families (whose breadwinners had professional occupations such as dentist).

“In addition:

2. Many of the adolescent cannabis users in the Dunedin study were from slightly lower SES homes, though not all.

3. However, their IQs were not boosted by schooling, because their IQ scores are the same from the time they began school to adolescence.

4. In a critical test, low SES is statistically unrelated to IQ decline from adolescence to adulthood. Therefore, statistical control for SES does not alter the findings we reported in our paper.

5. We add that there is no pattern of cannabis use predicting greater decline on the sections of the IQ test that are known to be most susceptible to alteration by education or social environment. We reported decline across many different kinds of mental functions.”

Professor Richie Poulton is Director of the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, University of Otago, and co-author of the original research.

*Rogeberg, O. Correlations between cannabis use and IQ change in the Dunedin cohort are consistent with confounding from socioeconomic status. PNAS Early Edition, 14–18 January 2013.
Source: AusSMC